Diferencias individuales en razonamiento hipotético-deductivoimportancia de la flexibilidad y de las habilidades cognitivas

  1. Seoane Pesqueira, Gloria
  2. Valiña García, María Dolores
  3. Rodríguez González, María Soledad
  4. Martín Rajo, Montserrat
  5. Ferraces Otero, María José
Revista:
Psicothema

ISSN: 0214-9915

Ano de publicación: 2007

Volume: 19

Número: 2

Páxinas: 206-211

Tipo: Artigo

Outras publicacións en: Psicothema

Resumo

Investigaciones recientes señalan que la ejecución diferencial de las personas cuando resuelven tareas de razonamiento se debe no sólo a diferencias a nivel computacional, sino también a nivel racional o intencional. Este trabajo se enmarca en este contexto y en él, concretamente, se analizan diferencias individuales en razonamiento hipotético-deductivo teniendo en cuenta, además de características asociadas a la propia tarea (contenido e instrucciones), características diferenciales de los propios sujetos. Participaron 276 estudiantes de la Universidad de Santiago de Compostela a los que se les aplicaron diferentes pruebas psicométricas y que resolvieron distintas tareas de razonamiento. Los resultados que hemos obtenido confirman que, efectivamente, las diferencias entre razonadores competentes y no competentes se dan en los dos niveles señalados: habilidades y capacidades cognitivas (nivel algorítmico) y flexibilidad cognitiva (nivel intencional).

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Anderson, J.R. (1990). The adaptative character of thought. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.
  • Anderson, J.R. (1991). Is human cognition adaptative? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 471-517.
  • Evans, J. St.B.T. (1984). Heuristic and analytic processes in reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 451-468.
  • Evans, J. St.B.T. (1989). Bias in human reasoning: Causes and consequences. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd.
  • Evans, J. St.B.T. (2003). In two minds: Dual process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(10), 545-459.
  • Evans, J. St.B.T. , Newstead, S.E., y Byrne, R.M.J. (1993). Human reasoning. The Psychology of deduction. Hove, UK: LEA.
  • Evans, J. St.B.T., y Over, D.E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. UK: Psychology Press.
  • Evans, J. St.B.T., Over, D.E., y Handley, S. (2003). A theory of hypothetical thinking. En D. Hardman y L. Maachi (eds.): Thinking. Psychological perspectives on reasoning, judgment and decision making (pp. 3- 21). England: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
  • Galián, M.D., Carranza, J.A., Escudero, A.J., Ato, M., y Ato, E. (2006). Diferencias individuales en la competencia lingüística de los sujetos diferenciales y expresivos. Psicothema, 18(1), 37-42.
  • Girotto, V., y Legrenzi, P. (1989). Mental representation and hypotheticodeductive reasoning: The case of the THOG problem. Psychological Research, 51, 129-135.
  • Goel, V., y Dolan, R.J. (2003). Explaining modulation of reasoning by belief. Cognition 87(1), B11-B22.
  • Griggs, R.A., y Newstead, S.E. (1982). The role of problem structure in a deductive reasoning task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 8, 297-307.
  • Griggs, R.A., Platt, R.D., Newstead, S.E., y Jackson, S.L. (1998). Attentional factors in a disjunctive reasoning task. Thinking and Reasoning, 4(1), 1-14.
  • Handley, S.J., Capon, A., Beveridge, M., Dennis, I., y Evans, St. B.T. (2004). Working memory, inhibitory control and the development of children’s reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning, 10, 123-146.
  • Johnson-Laird, P.N. (2000). The current state of the Mental Model Theory. En J.A. García-Madruga, N. Carriedo y Mª J. González-Labra (eds.): Mental Models in Reasoning (pp. 17-40). Varia, Madrid: UNED.
  • Johnson-Laird, P.N. (2001). Mental models and deduction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(10), 434-442.
  • Kahneman, D., y Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. En T. Gilovich, D. Griffin y D. Kahneman (eds.): Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 49-81). UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Klaczynski, P., y Daniel, D. (2005). Individual differences in conditional reasoning: A dual-process account. Thinking and Reasoning, 11(4), 305-325.
  • Koenig, C.S., y Griggs, R.A. (2004). Analogical transfer in the THOG task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 557-570.
  • Macizo, P., Bajo, T., y Soriano, M.F. (2006). Memoria operativa y control ejecutivo: procesos inhibitorios. Psicothema, 18(1), 112-166.
  • Marek, P., Griggs, R.A., y Koenig, C.S. (2000). Reducing cognitive complexity in a hypothetico-deductive reasoning task. Thinking and Reasoning, 6(3), 253-265.
  • Martín, M., y Valiña, Mª D. (2003). Dos décadas de investigación sobre el problema THOG: ¿Una disyunción por resolver? Revista de Psicología General y Aplicada, 56(1), 21-43.
  • Moshman, D. (1994). Reasoning, metareasoning and the promotion of rationality. En A. Demetriou y A. Efklides (eds.): Intelligence, mind and reasoning: Structure and development (pp. 135-150). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  • Needham, W.P., y Amado, C.A. (1995). Facilitation and transfer with narrative thematic versions of the THOG task. Psychological Research, 58, 67-73.
  • Newstead, S.E., Girotto, V., y Legrenzi, P. (1995). The THOG problem and its implications for human reasoning. En S.E. Newstead y Evans, J.St.B.T. (eds.): Perspectives on thinking and reasoning. Essays in honour of Peter Wason (pp. 261-285). Hove, U.K.: LEA.
  • Newstead, S.E., Handley, S.J, Harley, C., Wright, H., y Farrelly, D. (2004). Individual differences in deductive reasoning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 33-60.
  • O’Brien, D.P., Noveck, J.A., Davidson, G.M., Fish, S.M., Lea, R.B., y Freitag, J. (1990). Sources of difficulty in deductive reasoning: The THOG task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42, 329-351.
  • Roberts, M.J., y Newton, E.J. (eds.) (2005). Methods of thought. Individual differences in reasoning strategies. Hove: Psychology Press.
  • Ruff, Ch.C., Knauff, M., Fangmeier, Th., y Spreer, J. (2003). Reasoning and working memory: Common and distinct neuronal processes. Neuropsychologia, 41(9), 1241-1253.
  • Sá, W.C., West, R.F., y Stanovich, K.E. (1999). The domain specificity and generality of belief bias: Searching for a generalizable critical thinking skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 497-510.
  • Seisdedos, N., y Carrasco, J. (2000). BAMI. Batería de aptitudes mentales informatizada. Madrid: TEA.
  • Seoane, G., Rodríguez, M.S., y Valiña, M.D. (2002). Diferencias individuales en razonamiento condicional: predictores psicométricos. Metodología de las Ciencias del Comportamiento, volumen especial, 509- 512.
  • Seoane, G., Valiña, Mª D., Ferraces, Mª J., y Martín, M. (1997, July). Comparing measures of individual differences in performance of conditional reasoning. Proceedings of the 10th European Meeting of the Psychometric Society. Santiago de Compostela, Spain.
  • Shafir, E. (1994). Uncertainty and the difficulty of thinking through disjunctions. Cognition, 50, 403-430.
  • Stanovich, K.E. (1999). Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Stanovich, K.E. (2002). Rationality, intelligence and levels of analysis in Cognitive Science: Is dysrationalia possible? En R.J. Sternberg (ed.): Why smart people can be so stupid? (pp. 124-158). New Haven: Yale University Press.
  • Stanovich, K.E. (2004). The robot’s rebellion. Finding meaning in the age of Darwin. Chicago and London: The University of Chicago Press.
  • Stanovich, K.E., Sá, W.C., y West. R.F. (2004). Individual differences in thinking, reasoning and decision making. En J.P. Leighton y R.J. Sternberg (eds.): The Nature of Reasoning (pp. 375-409). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Stanovich, K.E., y West, R.F. (1997). Reasoning independently of prior belief and individual differences in actively open-minded thinking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 342-357.
  • Stanovich, K.E., y West, R.F. (1998). Individual differences in rational thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 161-188.
  • Stanovich, K.E., y West, R.F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645-726.
  • Stanovich, K.E., y West, R.F. (2003). Evolutionary versus instrumental goals: How evolutionary psychology misconceives human rationality. En D.E. Over (ed.): Evolution and the psychology of thinking. The debate (pp. 171-230). Hove and New York: Psychology Press.
  • Stavy, R., Goel, V., Critchley, H., y Dolan, R. (in press). Intuitive interference in quantitative reasoning. Brain Research. Available online. 27 january 2006.
  • Toplak, M.E., y Stanovich, K.E. (2002). The domain specifity and generality of disjunctive reasoning: Searching for a generalizable critical thinking skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(1), 197-200.
  • Valiña, Mª D., Seoane, G., Ferraces, Mª J., y Martín, M. (1995). Tarea de selección de Wason: un estudio de las diferencias individuales. Psicothema, 7(3), 641-653
  • Valiña, Mª D., Seoane, G., Ferraces, Mª J., y Martín, M. (2000). Conditio- nal reasoning: The importance of individual differences. En J.A. García-Madruga, N. Carriedo y Mª J. González-Labra (eds.): Mental models in reasoning. (pp. 249-267). Madrid: Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia.
  • Verschueren, N., Schaeken, W., y D’Ydewalle, G. (2005). A dual-process specification of causal conditional reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning, 11(3), 239-278.
  • Wason, P.C. (1977). Self-contradictions. En P.N. Johnson-Laird y P.C. Wason (eds.): Thinking: Readings in Cognitive Science (pp. 114-128). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Wason, P.C., y Brooks, P.G. (1979). THOG: The anatomy of a problem. Psychological Research, 41, 79-90.