‘I know what is best for you better than you do’cross-cultural evidence of the association between Monopoly on Truth and extremism

  1. Marcos Dono
  2. Mónica Alzate
  3. María-Idaly Barreto-Galeano
  4. Domenico Uhng-Hur
  5. José-Manuel Sabucedo
Revista:
International Journal of Social Psychology, Revista de Psicología Social

ISSN: 0213-4748 1579-3680

Ano de publicación: 2022

Volume: 37

Número: 3

Páxinas: 504-528

Tipo: Artigo

Outras publicacións en: International Journal of Social Psychology, Revista de Psicología Social

Resumo

El extremismo político representa una amenaza colectiva en las sociedades abiertas y democráticas, un peligro que ha aumentado en épocas recientes. El objetivo principal de esta investigación es probar un nuevo constructo, el Monopolio de la Verdad (MoT), analizando su asociación con actos extremistas ilegales y violentos en un estudio transcultural. Con este fin, la asociación entre el MoT y el extremismo ha sido evaluada en Brasil, Colombia y en España (N = 785). Los resultados respaldan la invarianza configural y métrica de la escala MoT, apoyando la validez del constructo. Los datos obtenidos reproducen también la validación original, mostrando una relación curvilínea entre ideología y MoT. Crucialmente, el análisis correlacional muestra que el MoT está asociado con el extremismo en todos los países que participan en el estudio, incluso al controlar la ideología. Esta investigación contribuye a la literatura de los fundamentos psicológicos del extremismo, introduciendo un correlato nuevo a la acción política ilegal y violenta, basado en una noción de superioridad moral y sin sesgo con respecto a la ideología.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N. (1950). The authoritarian personality. Abridged Ed). WW Norton & Co.
  • Altemeyer, B. (1981). Right-wing authoritarianism. University of Manitoba press.
  • Alzate, M., Durán, M., & Sabucedo, J. M. (2009). Población civil y transformación constructiva de un conflicto armado interno: Aplicación al caso colombiano. Universitas Psychologica, 8(3), 703–720. https://revistas.javeriana.edu.co/index.php/revPsycho/article/view/617
  • Aronson, J., Cohen, G., & Nail, P. R. (1999). Self-affirmation theory: An update and appraisal. In E. Harmon-Jones & J. Mills (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology (pp. 127–147). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10318-006
  • Bandura, A. (2015). Moral disengagement: How people do harm and live with themselves. Macmillan Learning.
  • Berger, J. (2017). Extremist Construction of Identity: How Escalating Demands for Legitimacy Shape and Define In-Group and Out-Group Dynamics. The International Centre for Counter-Terrorism – The Hague. http://dx.doi.org/10.19165/2017.1.07
  • Bianconi, G. (2021, October 11). I capi di Forza Nuova, l’ex Nar Pantera: Così i neofascisti si mischiano alla piazza no vax. Corriere della Sera. https://www.corriere.it/cronache/21_ottobre_11/assalto-sede-cgil-roma-forza-nuova-castellino-fiore-biagio-passaro-aronica-pantera-e27941a0-29f5-11ec-bcd6-9bba3a47f3bc.shtml
  • Borum, R. (2011). Radicalization into violent extremism I: A review of social science theories. Journal of Strategic Security, 4(4), 7–36. https://doi.org/10.5038/1944-0472.4.4.1
  • Bötticher, A. (2017). Towards academic consensus definitions of radicalism and extremism. Perspectives on Terrorism, 11(4), 73–77. https://shibbolethsp.jstor.org/start?entityID=https%3A%2F%2Fwww.rediris.es%2Fsir%2Fuscidp&dest=https://www.jstor.org/stable/26297896&site=jstor
  • Brown, T. A. (2015). Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research (2nd ed.). The Guilford Press.
  • Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9(2), 233–255. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5
  • Conway, L. G., Houck, S. C., Gornick, L. J., & Repke, M. A. (2018). Finding the Loch Ness monster: Left-wing authoritarianism in the United States. Political Psychology, 39(5), 1049–1067. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12470
  • Davidov, E., Meuleman, B., Cieciuch, J., Schmidt, P., & Billiet, J. (2014). Measurement equivalence in cross-national research. Annual Review of Sociology, 40(1), 55–75. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-071913-043137
  • Dolz, P. O. (2021, September 20). Asociaciones vecinales de corte neonazi protagonizaron la manifestación homófoba de Chueca. El País. https://elpais.com/sociedad/2021-09-20/asociaciones-vecinales-de-corte-neonazi-protagonizaron-la-manifestacion-homofoba-de-chueca.html
  • Dono, M., Alzate, M., Seoane, G., & Sabucedo, J. M. (2018). Development and validation of the Monopoly on Truth Scale. A measure of political extremism. Psicothema, 30(3), 330–336. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2017.423
  • Duckitt, J., Bizumic, B., Krauss, S. W., & Heled, E. (2010). A tripartite approach to right-wing authoritarianism: The authoritarianism-conservatism-traditionalism model. Political Psychology, 31(5), 685–715. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00781.x
  • Eady, G., Nagler, J., Guess, A., Zilinsky, J., & Tucker, J. A. (2019). How many people live in political bubbles on social media? Evidence from linked survey and Twitter data. SAGE Open, 9(1), 2158244019832705. https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244019832705
  • Fiske, A. P., & Rai, T. S. (2014). Virtuous violence: Hurting and killing to create, sustain, end, and honor social relationships. Cambridge University Press.
  • Gent, W. P. C. V., Mamadouh, V. D., & Wusten, H. H. V. D. (2013). Political reactions to the euro crisis: Cross-national variations and rescaling issues in elections and popular protests. Eurasian Geography and Economics, 54(2), 135–161. https://doi.org/10.1080/15387216.2013.811362
  • Henrich, J. (2020). The weirdest people in the world: How the west became psychologically peculiar and particularly prosperous. Penguin.
  • Hogg, M. A., Kruglanski, A., & van den Bos, K. (2013). Uncertainty and the roots of extremism. Journal of Social Issues, 69(3), 407–418. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12021
  • Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339–375. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339
  • Kroh, M. (2007). Measuring left-right political orientation: The choice of response format. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 71(2), 204–220. JSTOR. https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfm009
  • Kruglanski, A. W., Gelfand, M. J., Bélanger, J. J., Sheveland, A., Hetiarachchi, M., & Gunaratna, R. (2014). The psychology of radicalization and deradicalization: How significance quest impacts violent extremism. Political Psychology, 35(S1), 69–93. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12163
  • Kruglanski, A. W. (2018). Violent radicalism and the psychology of prepossession. Social Psychological Bulletin, 13(4), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.32872/spb.v13i4.27449
  • Kruglanski, A. W., Fernandez, J. R., Factor, A. R., & Szumowska, E. (2019). Cognitive mechanisms in violent extremism. Cognition, 188, 116–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.11.008
  • McGrath, M. (2020, November 4). Climate change: US formally withdraws from Paris agreement. BBC News. https://www.bbc.com/news/science-environment-54797743
  • McIntyre, L. (2018). Post-truth. MIt Press.
  • Moskalenko, S., & McCauley, C. (2009). Measuring political mobilization: The distinction between activism and radicalism. Terrorism and Political Violence, 21(2), 239–260. https://doi.org/10.1080/09546550902765508
  • Popper, K. (1945). The open society and its enemies. Routledge.
  • Putnick, D. L., & Bornstein, M. H. (2016). Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: The state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Developmental Review : DR, 41, 71–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.004
  • Rokeach, M. (1960). The open and closed mind: Investigations into the nature of belief systems and personality systems. Martino Fine Books.
  • Sabucedo, J. M. (1985). Incertidumbre: Una nueva medida del autoritarismo de estilo. Análisis y modificación de conducta, 11(27), 73–84. https://doi.org/10.33776/amc.v11i27.1688
  • Sabucedo, J.-M., Dono, M., Alzate, M., & Seoane, G. (2018). The importance of protesters’ morals: Moral obligation as a key variable to understand collective action. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00418
  • Sabucedo, J. M., Alzate, M., & Gómez-Román, C. (2020). Extremismo e secessionismo en contextos de crise. O movemento independentista na Cataluña.: Vol. Psicologia dos extremismos politicos (Edited by D. Uhng Hur & J. M. Sabucedo; pp. 16–41). Vozes.
  • Schmitt, N., & Kuljanin, G. (2008). Measurement invariance: Review of practice and implications. Human Resource Management Review, 18(4), 210–222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.hrmr.2008.03.003
  • Schönbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality, 47(5), 609–612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.05.009
  • Schraer, R., & Morrison, N. (2021). Covid: Misleading vaccine claims target children and parents. BBC News. https://www.bbc.com/news/health-58783711
  • Shils, E. A. (1954). Authoritarianism: “Right and left. In R. Christie & M. Jahoda (Eds.), Studies in the scope and method of the authoritarian personality (pp. 24–49). The Free Press New York.
  • Skitka, L. J. (2002). Do the means always justify the ends, or do the ends sometimes justify the means? A value protection model of justice reasoning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(5), 588–597. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167202288003
  • Skitka, L. J. (2010). The psychology of moral conviction. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(4), 267–281. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00254.x
  • Smith, P. (2021). Far right spies an opportunity in Europe’s new wave of Covid pain and protest. NBC News. https://www.nbcnews.com/news/world/far-right-spies-opportunity-europes-new-wave-covid-pain-protest-rcna5695
  • Vandenberg, R. J., & Lance, C. E. (2000). A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 3(1), 4–70. https://doi.org/10.1177/109442810031002
  • Vilas, X., & Sabucedo, J.-M. (2012). Moral obligation: A forgotten dimension in the analysis of collective action. International Journal of Social Psychology, 27(3), 369–375. https://doi.org/10.1174/021347412802845577
  • Wang, S., Chen, C.-C., Dai, C.-L., & Richardson, G. B. (2018). A call for, and beginner’s guide to, measurement invariance testing in evolutionary psychology. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 4(2), 166–178. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-017-0125-5
  • Webber, D., Babush, M., Schori-Eyal, N., Vazeou-Nieuwenhuis, A., Hettiarachchi, M., Bélanger, J. J., Moyano, M., Trujillo, H. M., Gunaratna, R., Kruglanski, A. W., & Gelfand, M. J. (2018). The road to extremism: Field and experimental evidence that significance loss-induced need for closure fosters radicalization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114(2), 270–285. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000111
  • Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 1049. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.67.6.1049
  • Williams, R. M. (1981). Legitimate and illegitimate uses of violence. In W. Gaylin, R. Macklin, & T. M. Powledge (Eds.), Violence and the politics of research (pp. 23–45). Springer US. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-4019-5_2
  • The World Bank. (2018). GINI 2018 (World Development Indicators). https://databank.worldbank.org/data/reports.aspx?source=2&series=SI.POV.GINI&country=WLD