Psychology and Tortureenhanced Interrogation Techniques in the Aftermath of the 9/11 Attacks

  1. Álvarez-Cruz, Antonio 1
  1. 1 Universidade de Santiago de Compostela
    info

    Universidade de Santiago de Compostela

    Santiago de Compostela, España

    ROR https://ror.org/030eybx10

Journal:
Revista de historia de la psicología

ISSN: 0211-0040 2445-0928

Year of publication: 2021

Volume: 42

Issue: 3

Pages: 2-9

Type: Article

More publications in: Revista de historia de la psicología

Abstract

In the aftermath of the September 2001 attacks in the USA, many suspects were arrested, kept in custody and interrogated by several US government agencies in order to obtain information about terrorist groups. Significant evidence indicates that some of the methods used, so-called “enhanced interrogation techniques”, included torture, and points to psychologists being involved in these practices. This article looks into this involvement as follows: firstly, the creation of the Behavioral Science Consultation Teams in detention centers such as Guantanamo; secondly, the variety of techniques employed to “enhance” interrogation, many of which had been designed, and even implemented by psychologists; thirdly, the successive efforts carried out by the American Psychological Association (APA) to respond to the public outcry provoked by their involvement in these practices; finally, motives for the widespread acceptability of torture, and motives that deny its effectiveness for obtaining relevant information.

Bibliographic References

  • Amnesty International. (2014). Torture in 2014: 30 years of broken promises. Retrieved from 01 (amnesty.org)
  • Arrigo, J. M. & Wagner, R. V. (2007). Psychologists and military interrogators rethink the psychology of torture. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 13(4), 393-398. https://doi.org/10.1080/10781910701665550
  • Arrizabalaga Clemente, P. (2007). Primum non nocere. Una reflexión sobre la aplicación del juramento hipocrático en la organización de las empresas sanitarias [A reflection on the application of the hippocratic oath for the organization of sanitary companies]. Medicina Clínica, 128(5), 181-3. Retrieved from http://www.doyma.es
  • Bagaric, M. & Clarke, J. (2005). Not enough official torture in the world? The circumstances in which torture is morally justifiable. University of San Francisco Law Review, 39(3), 581-616. Available from Deakin Research Online: http://hdl.handle.net/10536/DRO/DU:30003363
  • Bloche, M. G. & Marks, J. H. (2005). Doctors and interrogators at Guantanamo Bay. The New England Journal of Medicine, 353, 6-8. doi:10.1056/NEJMp058145
  • Eidelson, R. J. (2015). “No cause for action”: Revisiting the ethics case of Dr. John Leso. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 3(1), 198-212. doi:10.5964/ jspp.v3i1.479
  • García Amado, J. A. (2016). ¿Puede la tortura estar moralmente justificada en algún caso? [Can torture be morally justified?] Revista Nuevo Foro Penal, 12(86), 13-61. doi: https://doi.org/10.17230/nfp.12.86.1
  • Gray, K. & Wegner, D. M. (2010). Torture and judgments of guilt. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 233-235. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.10.003
  • Harper, D. (2007). The complicity of psychology in the Security State. In R. Roberts (ed.), Just war: psychology and terrorism. Ross-on-Wye, UK: PCCS Books.
  • Hausman, K. (2006). Military looks to psychologists for advice on interrogation. Psychiatric News, 41. Retrieved from https://psychnews.psychiatryonline. org/doi/full/10.1176/pn.41.13.0004
  • Heron, W. (1957). The pathology of boredom. Scientific American, 196, 52-56. Retrieved from The Pathology of Boredom (wordpress.com)
  • Hoffman, D. H., Carter, D. J., Lopez, C. R. V., Benzmiller, H. L., Guo, A. X., Latifi, S. Y. & Craig, D. C. (2015). Report to the Special Committee of the Board of Directors of the American Psychological Association: Independent review relating to APA Ethics Guidelines, national security interrogations, and torture. Chicago, IL: Sidley Austin LLP. Retrieved from https://www.apa.org/independentreview/APA-FINAL-Report-7.2.15.pdf
  • Homant, R. J. & Witkowski, M. J. (2011). Support for coercive interrogation among college students: Torture and the ticking bomb scenario. Journal of Applied Security Research, 6, 135-157. doi: 10.1080/19361610.2011.552002
  • Houck, S. C. & Repke, M. A. (2017). When and why we torture: A review of psychology research. Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 3(3), 272- 283. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/tps0000120
  • Kahneman, D. (2012). Pensar rápido, pensar despacio. [Thinking, fast and slow] Barcelona: Debate.
  • Koocher, G. (2006). Speaking against torture. Monitor on Psychology. Retrieved from https://www.apa.org/monitor/feb06/pc
  • Leahey, T. H. (2013). A history of psychology: from antiquity to modernity, 7th Edition. Boston: Pearson.
  • Lewis, N. A. (2004). Red Cross finds detainee abuse in Guantánamo. New York Times. Retrieved from Red Cross Finds Detainee Abuse in Guantánamo - The New York Times (nytimes.com)
  • Mayer, J. (2005). The Experiment. The New Yorker. Retrieved from http://www. newyorker.com/magazine/2005/07/11/the-experiment-3
  • Mayer, J. (2005b). A deadly interrogation: Can the C.I.A. legally kill a prisoner? The New Yorker. Retrieved from A Deadly Interrogation | The New Yorker
  • Miller, A. G. (2004) Introduction and overview. In A. G. Miller (ed.), The social psychology of good and evil (pp. 1-17). New York: The Guilford Press.
  • Mollica, R. F. (2004). Surviving torture. The New England Journal of Medicine, 351(1), 5-7. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp048141
  • O’Mara, S. (2018). The captive brain: torture and the neuroscience of humane interrogation. QJM: An International Journal of Medicine, 73-78. doi: 10.1093/ qjmed/hcx252
  • Pérez, J. (2012). Breve historia de la Inquisición en España (Brief history of the Inquisition in Spain]. Barcelona: Crítica.
  • Pérez-Sales, P. (2016). Tortura psicológica: Definición, evaluación y medida [Psychological torture: Definition, evaluation, and measurement]. Bilbao: Desclée de Brouwer.
  • SSCI. United States Senate Select Committee on Intelligence. (2014). Committee study of the Central Intelligence Agency’s detention and interrogation program. Retrieved from http://www.intelligence.senate.gov/publications/ committee-study-central-intelligence-agencys-detention-andinterrogation-program
  • Soufan, A. (2020). The Black Banners (Declassified): How Torture Derailed the War on Terror after 9/11. W. W. Norton & Company.
  • Staub, E. (1989). The roots of evil: The origins of genocide and other group violence. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Summers, F. (2007). Psychoanalysis, the American Psychological Association, and the involvement of psychologists at Guantanamo Bay. Psychoanalysis, Culture, and Society, 12(1), 83-92. doi.org/10.1057/palgrave.pcs.2100114
  • Waller, J. (2002). Becoming evil: How ordinary people commit genocide and mass killing. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Wessells, M. (2006). Letters. Monitor on Psychology. Retrieved from https:// www.apa.org/monitor/may06/letters
  • Zegart, A. B. (2010). “Spytainment”: the real influence of fake spies. International Journal of Intelligence and CounterIntelligence, 23(4), 599-622. dx.doi.org/10.1 080/08850607.2010.501635
  • Zimbardo, P. G. (2004). A situationist perspective on the psychology of evil: Understanding how good people are transformed into perpetrators. In A. G. Miller (ed.), The social psychology of good and evil (pp. 21-50). New York: The Guilford Press.
  • Zimbardo, P. G. (2008). El efecto Lucifer: El porqué de la maldad. [The Lucifer effect: How good people turn evil]. Barcelona: Paidós