El papel del ADN fósil en paleoantropologíaFoxp2, neandertales y lenguaje

  1. Benítez Burraco, Antonio
  2. Longa Martínez, Víctor Manuel
Revista:
Zephyrus: Revista de prehistoria y arqueología

ISSN: 0514-7336

Ano de publicación: 2011

Número: 67

Páxinas: 45-68

Tipo: Artigo

Outras publicacións en: Zephyrus: Revista de prehistoria y arqueología

Resumo

Fossil DNA analysis may become an important source of evidence in Paleoanthropology, in the sense that it may throw light on the inconclusiveness of more traditional evidence that discipline is concerned with (fossil reconstructions, etc.). However, caution is in order: if paleogenetic evidence is not rightly understood, inferences brought to the fore from it could be as ambiguous as the more traditional body of evidence. Our paper makes the point that prudence is required when trying to draw inferences about behavior or cognition from the fossil DNA analysis. We illustrate such a point by discussing the recent discovery of the modern FOPX2 gene version in Neanderthals. Many paleoanthropologists have taken that discovery to be the definitive proof of the existence of complex language in that species. We will aim at showing that the aforementioned inference cannot be automatically established: it is based on a simplistic and reductionist view, which assumes, among many other aspects, that genes are simple causal agents, and that a direct link does exist between genes and characters.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • BAGUÑÁ, J. y GARCÍA-FERNÁNDEZ, J. (2003): "Evo-Devo: The long and winding road", International Journal of Developmental Biology, 47, pp. 705-713.
  • BAKER, M. C. (2001): The atoms of language. The mind's hidden rules of grammar. New York: Basic Books.
  • BALABAN, E. (2000): "Behavior genetics: Galen's prophecy or Malpighi's legacy?". En SINGH, R.; KRIMBAS, C.; PAUL, D. y BEATTY, J. (eds.): Thinking about evolution: Historical, philosophical, and political perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 429-466.
  • BALABAN, E. (2006): "Cognitive developmental biology: History, process and fortune's wheel", Cognition, 101, pp. 298-332.
  • BALARI, S.; BENÍTEZ BURRACO, A.; CAMPS, M.; LONGA, V. M.; LORENZO, G. y URIAGEREKA, J. (2011): "The archaeological record speaks: Bridging Anthropology and Linguistics", International Journal of Evolutionary Biology, vol. 2011, doi: 10.4061/2011/382679.
  • BALARI, S.; BENÍTEZ BURRACO, A.; LONGA, V. M. y LORENZO, G. (en prensa): "The fossils of language: What are they, who has them, how did they evolve?". En BOECKX, C. y GROHMANN, K. K. (eds.): Handbook of Biolinguistics. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • BALARI, S. y LORENZO, G. (2009): "Computational phenotypes: Where the theory of computation meets evo devo", Biolinguistics, 3, pp. 2-60.
  • BAR-YOSEF, O. (2008): "Can Paleolithic stone artifacts serve as evidence for prehistoric language?". En BENGTSON, J. (ed.): In hot pursuit of Prehistory. Essays in the four fields of Anthropology in honor of Harold Crane Fleming. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 373-379.
  • BATESON, P. (2001): "Behavioral development and Darwinian evolution". En OYAMA, S.; GRIFFITHS, P. E. y GRAY, R. D. (eds.): Cycles of contingencies. Developmental systems and evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 149-166.
  • BENÍTEZ BURRACO, A. (2005a): "FOXP2: del trastorno específico a la biología molecular del lenguaje. I. Aspectos etiológicos, neuroanatómicos, neurofisiológicos y moleculares", Revista de Neurología, 40/11, pp. 671-682.
  • BENÍTEZ BURRACO, A. (2005b): "FOXP2: del trastorno específico a la biología molecular del lenguaje. II. Implicaciones para la ontogenia y la filogenia del lenguaje", Revista de Neurología, 41/1, pp. 37-44.
  • BENÍTEZ BURRACO, A. (2008a): "FOXP2 y la biología molecular del lenguaje: nuevas evidencias. I. Aspectos fenotípicos y modelos animales", Revista de Neurología, 46/5, pp. 289-298.
  • BENÍTEZ BURRACO, A. (2008b): "FOXP2 y la biología molecular del lenguaje: nuevas evidencias. II. Aspectos moleculares e implicaciones para la ontogenia y la filogenia del lenguaje", Revista de Neurología, 46/6, pp. 351-359.
  • BENÍTEZ BURRACO, A. (2009): Genes y lenguaje: aspectos ontogenéticos, filogenéticos y cognitivos. Barcelona: Reverté.
  • BENÍTEZ BURRACO, A.; LONGA, V. M.; LORENZO, G. y URIAGEREKA, J. (2008): "Also sprach Neanderthalsis⋯ Or did she?", Biolinguistics, 2/2-3, pp. 225-232.
  • BERMÚDEZ DE CASTRO, J. M. a (2010): La evolución del talento. Cómo nuestros orígenes determinan nuestro presente. Barcelona: Debate.
  • BICKERTON, D. (1990): Language and species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • BINFORD, L. R. (1989): Debating Archaeology. San Diego: Academic Press.
  • BOË, L.-J.; HEIM, J.-L.; HONDA, K. y MAEDA, S. (2002): "The potential Neanderthal vowel space was as large as that of modern humans", Journal of Phonetics, 30/3, pp. 465-484.
  • BÖE, L.-J.; HEIM, J.-L.; HONDA, K.; MAEDA, S.; BADIN, P. y ABRY, C. (2007): "The vocal tract of newborn humans and Neanderthals. Acoustic capabilities and consequences for the debate on the origin of language. A reply to Lieberman (2007)", Journal of Phonetics, 35/4, pp. 564-581.
  • BOË, L.-J.; MAEDA, S. y HEIM, J.-L. (1999): "Neanderthal man was not morphologically handicapped for speech", Evolution of Communication, 3/1, pp. 49-77.
  • BOTHA, R. (2009): "Theoretical underpinnings of inferences about language evolution: the syntax used at Blombos Cave". En BOTHA, R. y KNIGHT, C. (eds.): The cradle of language. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 93-111.
  • BOTHA, R. y KNIGHT, C. (eds.) (2009): The cradle of language. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • BRUCE, H. A. y MARGOLIS, R. L. (2002): "FOXP2: novel exons, splice variants, and CAG repeat length stability", Human Genetics, 111, pp. 136-144.
  • CARROLL, S. B. (2005): Endless forms most beautiful. The new science of Evo Devo and the making of the animal kingdom. New York: W. W. Norton.
  • CARRUTHERS, P. (2002): "The cognitive functions of language", Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25/6, pp. 657-674.
  • CHOMSKY, N. (1968): Language and mind. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
  • CHOMSKY, N. (1980): Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press.
  • CHRISTIANSEN, M. H. y KIRBY, S. (2003): "Language evolution: The hardest problem in science?". En CHRISTIANSEN, M. H. y KIRBY, S. (eds.): Language evolution. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 1-15.
  • CLAESSENS, L. P. A. M.; O'CONNOR, P. M. y UNWIN, D. M. (2009): "Respiratory evolution facilitated the origin of pterosaur flight and aerial gigantism", PLoS One 4/2, e4497, doi: 10/1371/journal.pone.0004497.
  • COOLIDGE, F. L. y WYNN, T. (2004): "A cognitive and neuropsychological perspective on the Châtelperronian", Journal of Anthropological Research, 60, pp. 55-73.
  • DENNETT, D. C. (1996): Kinds of minds. Toward an understanding of consciousness. New York: Basic Books.
  • D'ERRICO, F. (2003): "The invisible frontier: A multispecies model for the origin of behavioral modernity", Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, pp. 188-202.
  • D'ERRICO, F. (2008): "The archaeology of language origin". En SMITH, A.; SMITH, K. y FERRER I CANCHO, R. (eds.): The evolution of language. Proceedings of EVOLANG7. Singapur: World Scientific Publishing, pp. 413-414.
  • D'ERRICO, F. (2009): "The archaeology of early religious practices: a plea for a hypothesis-testing approach". En RENFREW, C. y MORLEY, I. (eds.): Becoming human. Innovation in prehistoric material and spiritual culture. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 104-122.
  • D'ERRICO, F.; HENSHILWOOD, C.; VANHAEREN, M. y VAN NIEKERK, K. (2005): "Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave: evidence for symbolic behaviour in the Middle Stone Age", Journal of Human Evolution, 48, pp. 3-24.
  • D'ERRICO, F. y VANHAEREN, M. (2009): "Earliest personal ornaments and their significance for the origin of language debate". En BOTHA, R. y KNIGHT, C. (eds.): The cradle of language. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 16-40.
  • D'ERRICO, F.; ZILHÃO, J.; JULIEN, M.; BAFFIER, D. y PELEGRIN, J. (1998): "Neanderthal acculturation in Western Europe? A critical review of the evidence and its interpretation", Current Anthropology, 39/2, pp. S1-S22. Special issue: The Neanderthal problem and the evolution of human behavior.
  • ENARD, W.; GEHRE, S.; HAMMERSCHMIDT, K.; HÖLTER, S. M.; BLASS, T.; SOMEL, M.; BRÜCKNER, M. K.; SCHREIWEIS, C.; WINTER, C.; SOHR, R.; BECKER, L.; WIEBE, V.; NICKEL, B.; GIGER, T.; MÜLLER, U.; GROSZER, M.; ADLER, T.; AGUILAR, A.; BOLLE, I.; CALZADA-WACK, J.; DALKE, C.; EHRHARDT, N.; FAVO R, J.; FUCHS, H.; GAILUS-DURNER, V.; HANS, W.; HÖLZLWIMMER, G.; JAVAHERI, A.; KALAYDJIEV, S.; KALLNIK, M.; KLING, E.; KUNDER, S.; MOSSBRUGGER, I.; NATO N, B.; RACZ, I.; RATHKOLB, B.; ROZMAN, J.; SCHREWE, A.; BUSCH, D. H.; GRAW, J.; IVANDIC, B.; KLINGENSPOR, M.; KLOPSTOCK, T.; OLLERT, M.; QUINTANILLA- MARTÍNEZ, L.; SCHULZ, H.; WOLF, E.; WURST, W.; ZIMMER, A.; FISHER, S. E.; MORGENSTERN, R.; ARENDT, T.; DE ANGELIS, M. H.; FISCHER, J.; SCHWARZ, J. y PÄÄBO, S. (2009): "A humanized version of Foxp2 affects cortico-basal ganglia circuits in mice", Cell, 137, pp. 961-971.
  • ENARD, W.; KHAITOVICH, P.; KLOSE, J.; ZÖLLNER, S.; HEISSIG, F.; GIAVALISCO, P.; NIESELT-STRUWE, K.; MUCHMORE, E.; VARKI, A.; RAVID, R.; DOXIADIS, G. M.; BONTROP, R. E. y PÄÄBO, S. (2002b): "Intraand interspecific variation in primate gene expression patterns", Science, 296, pp. 340-343.
  • ENARD, W.; PRZEWORSKI, M.; FISHER, S. E.; LAI, C. S.; WIEBE, V.; KITANO, T.; MONACO, A. P. y PÄÄBO, S. (2002a): "Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language", Nature, 418, pp. 869-872.
  • FISHER, S. E. (2005): "Dissection of molecular mechanisms underlying speech and language disorders", Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, pp. 111-128.
  • FISHER, S. E. (2006): "Tangled webs: Tracing the connections between genes and cognition", Cognition, 101, pp. 270-297.
  • FISHER, S. E. y SCHARFF, C. (2009): "FOXP2 as a molecular window into speech and language", Trends in Genetics, 25/4, pp. 166-177.
  • FISHER, S. E.; VARGHA-KHADEM, F.; WATKINS, K.; MONACO, A. P. y PEMBREY, M. (1998): "Localization of a gene implicated in a severe speech and language disorder", Nature Genetics, 18, pp. 168-170.
  • FITCH, W. T. (2000): "The phonetic potential of nonhuman vocal tracts: comparative cineradiographic observations of vocalizing animals", Phonetica, 57, pp. 205-218.
  • FITCH, W. T. (2002): "Comparative vocal production and the evolution of speech: reinterpreting the descent of the larynx". En WRAY, A. (ed.): The transition to language. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 21-45.
  • FITCH, W. T. (2005): "The evolution of language: a comparative review", Biology and Philosophy, 20, pp. 193-230.
  • FITCH, W. T. (2009): "Fossil cues to the evolution of speech". En BOTHA, R. y KNIGHT, C. (eds.): The cradle of language. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 112-134.
  • FITCH, W. T. y REBY, D. (2001): "The descended larynx is not uniquely human", Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 268, pp. 1669-1675.
  • FRAYER, D. W.; FIORE, I.; LALUEZA-FOX, C.; RADOVCIC, J. y BONDIOLI, L. (2010): "Right handed Neandertals: Vindija and beyond", Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 88, pp. 113-127.
  • FRAYER, D. W.; LOZANO, M.; BERMÚDEZ DE CASTRO, J. M.; CARBONELL, E.; ARSUAGA, J. L.; RADOVCIC, J.; FIORE, I. y BONDIOLI, L. (2011): "More than 500, 000 years of right-handedness in Europe", Laterality, doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2010.529451.
  • FREY, R. y RIEDE, T. (2003): "Sexual dimorphism of the larynx of the Mongolian Gazelle (Procapra gutturosa Pallas, 1777) (Mammalia, Artiodactyla, Bovidae)", Zoologischer Anzeiger, 242, pp. 33-62.
  • FUJITA, E.; TANABE, Y.; SHIOTA, A.; UEDA, M.; SUWA, K.; MOMOI, M. Y. y MOMOI, T. (2008): "Ultrasonic vocalization impairment of Foxp2 (R552H) knockin mice related to speech-language disorder and abnormality of Purkinje cells", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, pp. 3117-3122.
  • GINGERAS, T. R. (2009): "Implications of chimaeric nonco-linear transcripts", Nature, 461, pp. 206-211.
  • GOLOVANOVA, L. V.; HOFFECKER, J. F.; KHARITONOV, V. M. y ROMANOVA, G. P. (1999): "Mezmaiskaya Cave: A Neanderthal occupation in the Northern Caucasus", Current Anthropology, 40/1, pp. 77-86.
  • GOODMAN, C. S. y COUGHLIN, B. C. (2000): "Introduction. The evolution of evo-devo biology", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, pp. 4424-4425.
  • GOODWIN, B. (1994): How the leopard changed its spots. The evolution of complexity. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons. Citamos por Las manchas del leopardo. La evolución de la complejidad. Barcelona: Tusquets, 1998.
  • GOPNIK, M. (1990): "Feature-blind grammar and dysphasia", Nature, 344, p. 715.
  • GRÄFF, J. y MANSUY, I. M. (2008): "Epigenetic codes in cognition and behaviour", Behavioural Brain Research, 192, pp. 70-87.
  • GREEN, R. E.; KRAUSE, J.; BRIGGS, A. W.; MARICIC, T.; STENZEL, U.; KIRCHER, M.; PATTERSON, N.; LI, H.; ZHAI, W.; FRITZ, M. H.; HANSEN, N. F.; DURAND, E. Y.; MALASPINAS, A. S.; JENSEN, J. D.; MARQUES-BONET, T.; ALKAN, C.; PRÜFER, K.; MEYER, M.; BURBANO, H. A.; GOOD, J. M.; SCHULTZ, R.; AXIMU-PETRI, A.; BUTTHOF, A.; HÖBER, B.; HÖFFNER, B.; SIEGEMUND, M.; WEIHMANN, A.; NUSBAUM, C.; LANDER, E. S.; RUSS, C.; NOVOD, N.; AFFOURTIT, J.; EGHOLM, M.; VERNA, C.; RUDAN, P.; BRAJKOVIC, D.; KUCAN, Z.; GUSIC, I.; DORONICHEV, V. B.; GOLOVANOVA, L. V.; LALUEZA-FOX, C.; DE LA RASILLA, M.; FORTEA, J.; ROSAS, A.; SCHMITZ, R. W.; JOHNSON, P. L.; EICHLER, E. E.; FALUSH, D.; BIRNEY, E.; MULLIKIN, J. C.; SLATKIN, M.; NIELSEN, R.; KELSO, J.; LACHMANN, M.; REICH, D. y PÄÄBO, S. (2010): "A draft sequence of the neandertal genome", Science, 328, pp. 710-722.
  • GRIFFITHS, P. E. y GRAY, R. D. (2004): "The developmental systems perspective: organism-environment systems as units of evolution". En PRESTON, K. y PIGLIUCCI, M. (eds.): Phenotypic integration: Studying the ecology and evolution of complex phenotypes. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 409-431.
  • GROSZER, M.; KEAYS, D. A.; DEACON, R. M.; DE BONO, J. P.; PRASAD-MULCARE, S.; GAUB, S.; BAUM, M. G.; FRENCH, C. A.; NICOD, J.; COVENTRY, J. A.; ENARD, W.; FRAY, M.; BROWN, S. D.; NOLAN, P. M.; PÄÄBO, S.; CHANNON, K. M.; COSTA, R. M.; EILERS, J.; EHRET, G.; RAWLINS, J. N. y FISHER, S. E. (2008): "Impaired synaptic plasticity and motor learning in mice with a point mutation implicated in human speech deficits", Current Biology, 18, pp. 354-362.
  • HAESLER, S.; ROCHEFORT, C.; GEORGI, B.; LICZNERSKI, P.; OSTEN, P. y SCHARFF, C. (2007): "Incomplete and inaccurate vocal imitation after knockdown of FoxP2 in songbird basal ganglia nucleus Area X", PLoS Biology, 5/12, e321, doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0050321.
  • HAESLER, S.; WADA, K.; NSHDEJAN, A.; MORRISEY, E. E.; LINTS, T.; JARVIS, E. D. y SCHARFF, C. (2004): "FoxP2 expression in avian vocal learners and nonlearners", Journal of Neuroscience, 24, pp. 3164-3175.
  • HALL, B. K. y OLSON, W. M. (2003): "Introduction: Evolutionary developmental mechanisms". En HALL, B. K. y OLSON, W. M. (eds.): Keywords & concepts in evolutionary developmental biology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. xiii-xvi.
  • HAUSER, M. D. (1996): The evolution of communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • HAUSER, M. D.; CHOMSKY, N. y FITCH, W. T. (2002): "The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve?", Science, 298, pp. 1569-1579.
  • HAUSER, M. D. y FITCH, W. T. (2003): "What are the uniquely human components of the language faculty?". En CHRISTIANSEN, M. y KIRBY, S. (eds.): Language evolution. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 158-181.
  • HENSHILWOOD, C. S.; D'ERRICO, F.; YATES, R.; JACOBS, Z.; TRIBOLO, C.; DULLER, G. A. T.; MERCIER, N.; SEALY, J. C.; VALLADAS, H.; WATTS, I. y WINTLE, A. G. (2002): "Emergence of modern human behaviour: Middle Stone Age engravings from South Africa", Science, 295, pp. 1278-1280.
  • HENSHILWOOD, C. S. y DUBREUIL, B. (2009): "Reading the artifacts: gleaning language skills from the Middle Stone Age in southern Africa". En BOTHA, R. y KNIGHT, C. (eds.): The cradle of language. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 41-61.
  • HENSHILWOOD, C. S. y MAREAN, C. W. (2003): "The origin of modern human behavior. Critique of the models and their test implications", Current Anthropology, 44, pp. 627-651.
  • HIGHAM, T.; JACOBI, R.; JULIEN, M.; DAVID, F.; BASELL, L.; WOOD, R.; DAVIES, W. y RAMSEY, C. B. (2010): "Chronology of the Grotte du Renne (France) and implications for the context of ornaments and human remains within the Châtelperronian", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107/47, pp. 20234-20239.
  • JABLONKA, E. y LAMB, M. J. (2005): Evolution in four dimensions. Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • JERISON, H. (1985): "Animal intelligence as encephalization". En WEISKRANTZ, L. (ed.): Animal intelligence. Oxford: Claredon Press, pp. 21-35.
  • JOHNSON, M. B.; KAWASAWA, Y. I.; MASON, C. E.; KRSNIK, Ž.; COPPOLA, G.; BOGDANOVIC, D.; GESCHWIND, D. H.; MANE, S. M.; STATE, M. W. y ŠESTAN, N. (2009): "Functional and evolutionary insights into human brain development through global transcriptome analysis", Neuron, 62, pp. 494-509.
  • KAY, L. E. (2000): Who wrote the book of life: A history of the genetic code. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
  • KHAITOVICH, P.; ENARD, W.; LACHMANN, M. y PÄÄBO, S. (2006): "Evolution of primate gene expression", Nature Reviews Genetics, 7, pp. 693-702.
  • KLEIN, R. G. (2009): The human career. 3 rd ed. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
  • KOJIMA, S. y KIRITANI, S. (1989): "Vocal-auditory functions in the chimpanzee: Vowel perception", International Journal of Primatology, 10/3, pp. 199-213.
  • KOJIMA, S.; TATSUMI, I.; KIRITANI, S. y HIROSE, H. (1989): "Vocal-auditory functions in the chimpanzee: Consonant perception", Human Evolution, 4/5, pp. 403-416.
  • KONOPKA, G.; BOMAR, J. M.; WINDEN, K.; COPPOLA, G.; JONSSON, Z. O.; GAO, F.; PENG, S.; PREUSS, T. M.; WOHLSCHLEGEL, J. A. y GESCHWIND, D. H. (2009): "Human-specific transcriptional regulation of CNS development genes by FOXP2", Nature, 462, pp. 213-217.
  • KRAUSE, J.; LALUEZA-FOX, C.; ORLANDO, L.; ENARD, W.; GREEN, R. E.; BURBANO, H. A.; HUBLIN, J.-J.; HÄNNI, C.; FORTEA, J.; DE LA RASILLA, M.; BERTRANPETIT, J.; ROSAS, A. y PÄÄBO, S. (2007): "The derived FOXP2 variant of modern humans was shared with Neandertals", Current Biology, 17, pp. 1908-1912.
  • KUHL, P. K. (1989): "On babies, birds, modules, and mechanisms: a comparative approach to the acquisition of vocal communication". En DOOLING, R. y HULSE, S. (eds.): The comparative psychology of audition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 379-422.
  • KUHL, P. K. y PADDEN, D. M. (1983): "Enhanced discriminability at the phonetic boundaries for the place feature in macaques", Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 73/3, pp. 1003-1010.
  • LAI, C. S.; FISHER, S. E.; HURST, J. A.; VARGHA-KHADEM, F. y MONACO, A. P. (2001): "A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder", Nature, 413, pp. 519-523.
  • LEVENSON, J. M. y SWEATT, J. D. (2006): "Epigenetic mechanisms: a common theme in vertebrate and invertebrate memory formation", Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences, 63/9, pp. 1009-1016.
  • LEWONTIN, R. C. (1998): "The evolution of cognition". En SCARBOROUGH, D. y STERNBERG, S. (eds.): An invitation to cognitive science. Vol. 4: Methods, models, and conceptual issues. 2 nd ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 107-132.
  • LI, G.; WANG, J.; ROSSITER, S. J.; JONES, G. y ZHANG, S. (2007): "Accelerated FoxP2 evolution in echolocating bats", PLoS ONE, 2/9, e900, doi:10.1371/journal. pone.0000900.
  • LI, S.; WEIDENFELD, J. y MORRISEY, E. E. (2004): "Transcriptional and DNA binding activity of the Foxp1/2/4 family is modulated by heterotypic and homotypic protein interactions", Molecular and Cellular Biology, 24, pp. 809-822.
  • LIEBERMAN, P. (2000): Human language and our reptilian brain. The subcortical bases of speech, syntax and thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • LIEBERMAN, P. (2002): "On the nature and evolution of the neural bases of human language", American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 45, pp. 36-62.
  • LIEBERMAN, P. (2003): "Motor control, speech, and the evolution of human language". En CHRISTIANSEN, M. y KIRBY, S. (eds.): Language evolution. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 255-271.
  • LIEBERMAN, P. (2006): Toward an evolutionary biology of language. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  • LIEBERMAN, P. (2007a): "The evolution of human speech. Its anatomical and neural bases", Current Anthropology, 48/1, pp. 39-66.
  • LIEBERMAN, P. (2007b): "Current views on Neanderthal speech capabilities: A reply to Boë et al. (2002)", Journal of Phonetics, 35/4, pp. 552-563.
  • LIEBERMAN, P. y CRELIN, E. S. (1971): "On the speech of Neanderthal man", Linguistic Inquiry, 2, pp. 203-222.
  • LIEBERMAN, P.; KLATT, D. H. y WILSON, W. H. (1969): "Vocal tract limitations on the vowel repertoires of rhesus monkey and other nonhuman primates", Science, 164, pp. 1185-1187.
  • LONGA, V. M. (2005): "Bibliografía comentada sobre la emergencia y la evolución del lenguaje (1990-2004)", Moenia, 11, pp. 423-458.
  • LONGA, V. M. (2006): "Sobre el significado del descubrimiento del gen FOXP2", Estudios de Lingüística. Universidad de Alicante, 20, pp. 177-207.
  • LUST, B. (2006): Child language: Acquisition and growth. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • MACNEILAGE, P. F. (1998): "The frame/content theory of evolution of speech production", Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, pp. 499-511.
  • MACNEILAGE, P. F. (2008): The origin of speech. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • MARCUS, G. y FISHER, S. E. (2003): "FOXP2 in focus: What can genes tell us about speech and language", Trends in Cognitive Science, 7/6, pp. 257-262.
  • MARTÍNEZ, I.; QUAM, R.; ROSA, M.; JARABO, P.; LORENZO, C. y ARSUAGA, J. L. (2008): "Auditory capacities of human fossils: a new approach to the origin of speech", Proceedings of the Conference Acoustics'08 Paris, pp. 4177-4182.
  • MARTÍNEZ, I.; ROSA, M.; ARSUAGA, J. L.; JARABO, P.; QUAM, R.; LORENZO, C.; GRACIA, A.; CARRETERO, J. M.; BERMÚDEZ DE CASTRO, J. M. y CARBONELL, E. (2004): "Auditory capacities in Middle Pleistocene humans from the Sierra de Atapuerca in Spain", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101/27, pp. 9976-9981.
  • MARTÍNEZ MENDIZÁBAL, I. y ARSUAGA, J. L. (2009): "El origen del lenguaje: la evidencia paleontológica", Munibe (Antropologia-Arkeologia), 60, pp. 5-16.
  • MATTICK, J. S.; TAFT, R. J. y FAULKNER, G. J. (2009): "A global view of genomic information: moving beyond the gene and the master regulator", Trends in Genetics, 26, pp. 21-28.
  • MAYNARD SMITH, J. (2000): "The concept of information in biology", Philosophy of Science, 67, pp. 177-194.
  • MAYNARD-SMITH, J. y SZATHMÁRY, E. (1995): The major transitions in evolution. Oxford y New York: W. H. Freeman.
  • MAYNARD-SMITH, J. y SZATHMÁRY, E. (1999): The origins of life. From the birth of life to the origin of language. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • MCBREARTY, S. y BROOKS, A. S. (2000): "The revolution that wasn't: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior", Journal of Human Evolution, 39, pp. 453-563.
  • MEHLER, M. F. (2008): "Epigenetic principles and mechanisms underlying nervous system functions in health and disease", Progress in Neurobiology, 86, pp. 305-341.
  • MELLARS, P. (1996): "Symbolism, language, and the Neanderthal mind". En MELLARS, P. y GIBSON, K. (eds.): Modelling the early human mind. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, pp. 15-32.
  • MELLARS, P. (1998): "Neanderthals, modern humans and the archaeological evidence for language". En JABLONSKI, N. y AIELLO, L. C. (eds.): The origin and diversification of language. San Francisco: California Academy of Sciences, pp. 89-115.
  • MELLARS, P. (2005): "The impossible coincidence. A single-species model for the origins of modern human behavior", Evolutionary Anthropology, 14, pp. 12-27.
  • MELLARS, P. (2010): "Neanderthal symbolism and ornament manufacture: The bursting of a bubble?", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107/47, pp. 20147-20148.
  • MITHEN, S. (1996): The prehistory of the mind. A search for the origins of art, religion, and science. London: Thames & Hudson.
  • MITHEN, S. (2006): The singing Neanderthals. The origins of music, language, mind and body. London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson. Citamos por Los neandertales cantaban rap. Los orígenes de la música y el lenguaje. Barcelona: Crítica, 2007.
  • MITHEN, S. (2007a): "How the evolution of the human mind might be reconstructed". En GANGESTAD, S. W. y SIMPSON, J. A. (eds.): The evolution of mind. Fundamental questions and controversies. New York: The Guilford Press, pp. 60-66.
  • MOCHIZUKI, A.; TAKEDA, Y. e IWASA, Y. (1996): "The evolution of genomic imprinting", Genetics, 144, pp. 1283-1295.
  • MOORE, B. C. J. (1989): An introduction to the psychology of hearing. 3 rd ed. London: Academic Press.
  • MÜLLER, G. B. (2007): "Evo-devo: extending the evolutionary synthesis", Nature Reviews Genetics, 8, pp. 943-949.
  • MÜLLER, G. B. y NEWMAN, S. A. (2005): "Evolutionary innovation and morphological novelty", Journal of Experimental Zoology Part B: Molecular and Developmental Evolution, 304, pp. 485-496.
  • NEGUS, V. E. (1949): The comparative anatomy and physiology of the larynx. New York: Hafner.
  • NEWMAN, S. A. y COMPER, W. D. (1990): "'Generic' physical mechanisms of morphogenesis and pattern formation", Development, 110, pp. 1-18.
  • NEWMAN, S. A.; FORGACS, G. y MÜLLER, G. B. (2006): "Before programs: the physical origination of multicellular forms", International Journal of Developmental Biology, 50, pp. 289-299.
  • OKASHA, S. (2006): Evolution and the levels of selection. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • OYAMA, S. (1985): The ontogeny of information. Developmental systems and evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hay 2. a ed. revisada, Durham: Duke University Press, 2000.
  • OYAMA, S.; GRIFFITHS, P. E. y GRAY, R. D. (eds.) (2001): Cycles of contingencies. Developmental systems and evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • PIATTELLI-PALMARINI, M. y URIAGEREKA, J. (2005): "The evolution of the narrow faculty of language: the skeptical view and a reasonable conjecture", Lingue e Linguaggio, IV/1, pp. 27-79.
  • PIATTELLI-PALMARINI, M. y URIAGEREKA, J. (2011): "A geneticist's dream, a linguist's nightmare: The case of FOXP2". En DI SCIULLO, A.-M. y BOECKX, C. (eds.): The biolinguistic enterprise. New perspectives on the evolution and nature of the human language faculty. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 100-125.
  • PICKLES, J. O. (1988): An introduction to the physiology of hearing. 2 nd ed. London: Academic Press.
  • PIGLIUCCI, M.; SCHLICHTING, C. D.; JONES, C. S. y SCHWENK, K. (1996): "Developmental reaction norms: the interactions among allometry, ontogeny, and plasticity", Plant Species Biology, 11, pp. 69-85.
  • PTAK, S.; ENARD, W.; WIEBE, V.; HELLMANN, I.; KRAUSE, J.; LACHMANN, M. y PÄÄBO, S. (2009): "Linkage disequilibrium extends across putative selected sites in FOXP2", Molecular Biology and Evolution, 26/10, pp. 2181-2184.
  • RAFF, R. A. (2000): "Evo-devo: The evolution of a new discipline", Nature Reviews Genetics, 1, pp. 74-79.
  • RAMEAU, J.-P. (1737): Génération harmonique, ou traité de musique théorique et pratique. Paris: Prault Fils.
  • ROBERT, J. S. (2008): "Taking old ideas seriously: Evolution, development, and human behavior", New Ideas in Psychology, 26, pp. 387-404.
  • ROEBROEKS, W. y VERPOORTE, A. (2009): "A 'languagefree' explanation for differences between the European Middle and Upper Paleolithic record". En BOTHA, R. y KNIGHT, C. (eds.): The cradle of language. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 150-166.
  • ROSAS, A. (2010): Los neandertales. Madrid: CSIC/Catarata.
  • ROSENBERG, A. (1997): "Reductionism redux: Computing the embryo", Biology and Philosophy, 12, pp. 445-470.
  • SATO, K.; SAKAMOTO, K. Q.; WATANUKI, Y.; TAKAHASHI, A.; KATSUMATA, N.; BOST, C.-A. y WEIMERSKIRCH, H. (2009): "Scaling of soaring seabirds and implications for flight abilities of giant pterosaurs", PLoS One, 4/4:e5400, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005400.
  • SCHAFFNER, K. F. (1998): "Genes, behavior, and developmental emergentism: One process, indivisible?", Philosophy of Science, 65, pp. 209-252.
  • SHOLTIS, S. y WEISS, K. M. (2005): "Phenogenetics: genotypes, phenotypes, and variation". En HALLGRÍMSSON, B. y HALL, B. K. (eds.): Variation. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 499-523.
  • SHRIBERG, L. D.; BALLARD, K. J.; TOMBLIN, J. B.; DUFFY, J. R.; ODELL, K. H. y WILLIAMS, C. A. (2006): "Speech, prosody, and voice characteristics of a mother and daughter with a 7, 13 translocation affecting FOXP2", Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, pp. 500-525.
  • SHU, W.; CHO, J. Y.; JIANG, Y.; ZHANG, M.; WEISZ, D.; ELDER, G. A.; SCHMEIDLER, J.; DE GASPERI, R.; GAMA SOSA, M. A.; RABIDOU, D.; SANTUCCI, A. C.; PERL, D.; MORRISEY, E. y BUXBAUM, J. D. (2005): "Altered ultrasonic vocalization in mice with a disruption in Foxp2 gene", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, pp. 9643-9648.
  • SHU, W.; YANG, H.; ZHANG, L.; LU, M. M. y MORRISEY, E. E. (2001): "Characterization of a new subfamily of wingedhelix/forkhead (Fox) genes that are expressed in the lung and act as transcriptional repressors", Journal of Biological Chemistry, 276, pp. 27488-27497.
  • SIKELA, J. M. (2006): "The jewels of our genome: the search for the genomic changes underlying the evolutionarily unique capacities of the human brain", PLoS Genetics, 2, e80.
  • SINGER, T.; MCCONNELL, M. J.; MARCHETTO, M. C. N.; COUFAL, N. G. y GAGE, F. H. (2010): "LINE-1 retrotransposons: mediators of somatic variation in neuronal genomes?", Trends in Neuroscience, 33/8, pp. 345-354.
  • SPITERI, E.; KONOPKA, G.; COPPOLA, G.; BOMAR, J.; OLDHAM, M.; OU, J.; VERNES, S. C.; FISHER, S. E.; REN, B. y GESCHWIND, D. H. (2007): "Identification of the transcriptional targets of FOXP2, a gene linked to speech and language, in developing human brain", American Journal of Human Genetics, 81, pp. 1144-1157.
  • TATTERSALL, I. (1998): Becoming human: Evolution and human uniqueness. New York: Harcourt Brace & Co.
  • TRINKAUS, E. (2007): "Human evolution: Neandertal gene speaks out", Current Biology, 17, pp. R917-R918.
  • TRUE, J. R. y CARROLL, S. B. (2002): "Gene co-option in physiological and morphological evolution", Annual Review of Cell and Developmental Biology, 18, pp. 53-80.
  • VALLENDER, E. J.; MEKEL-BOBROV, N. y LAHN, B. T. (2008): "Genetic basis of human brain evolution", Trends in Neuroscience, 31, pp. 637-644.
  • VARGHA-KHADEM, F.; GADIAN, D. G.; COPP, A. y MISHKIN, M. (2005): "FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language", Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, pp. 131-138.
  • VARGHA-KHADEM, F.; WATKINS, K. E.; ALCOCK, K. J.; FLETCHER, P. y PASSINGHAM, R. E. (1995): "Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92, pp. 930-933.
  • VARKI, A.; GESCHWIND, D. H. y EICHLER, E. E. (2008): "Explaining human uniqueness: genome interactions with environment, behaviour and culture", Nature Reviews Genetics, 9, pp. 749-763.
  • VERNES, S. C.; NEWBURY, D. F.; ABRAHAMS, B. S.; WINCHESTER, L.; NICOD, J.; GROSZER, M.; ALARCÓN, M.; OLIVER, P. L.; DAVIES, K. E.; GESCHWIND, D. H.; MONACO, A. P. y FISHER, S. E. (2008): "A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders", New England Journal of Medicine, 359, pp. 2337-2345.
  • VERNES, S. C.; NICOD, J.; ELAHI, F. M.; COVENTRY, J. A.; KENNY, N.; COUPE, A. M.; BIRD, L. E.; DAVIES, K. E. y FISHER, S. E. (2006): "Functional genetic analysis of mutations implicated in a human speech and language disorder", Human Molecular Genetics, 15, pp. 3154-3167.
  • VERNES, S. C.; SPITERI, E.; NICOD, J.; GROSZER, M.; TAYLOR, J. M.; DAVIES, K. E.; GESCHWIND, D. H. y FISHER, S. E. (2007): "High-throughput analysis of promoter occupancy reveals direct neural targets of FOXP2, a gene mutated in speech and language disorders", American Journal of Human Genetics, 81, pp. 1232-1250.
  • WALSH, D. (2007): "Development: three grades of ontogenetic involvement". En MATTHEN, M. y STEPHENS, C. (eds.): Handbook of the philosophy of science: Vol. 3, Philosophy of biology. Amsterdam: North-Holland, pp. 179-199.
  • WATKINS, K. E.; DRONKERS, N. F. y VARGHA-KHADEM, F. (2002): "Behavioural analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: comparison with acquired aphasia", Brain, 125, pp. 452-464.
  • WEAVER, T. D. y HUBLIN, J.-J. (2009): "Neanderthal birth canal shape and the evolution of human childbirth", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106/20, pp. 8151-8156.
  • WEISSENGRUBER, G. E.; FORSTENPOINTNER, G.; PETERS, G.; KÜBBER-HEISS, A. y FITCH, W. T. (2002): "Hyoid apparatus and pharynx in the lion (Panthera leo), jaguar (Panthera onca), tiger (Panthera tigris), cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus), and domestic cat (Felis silvestris f. catus)", Journal of Anatomy (London), 201, pp. 195-209.
  • WEST-EBERHARD, M. J. (2003): Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • WEST-EBERHARD, M. J. (2005): "Developmental plasticity and the origin of species differences", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(Supl. 1), pp. 6543-6549.
  • WHITE, S. A. (2011): "Genes and vocal learning", Brain and Language, 26/10, pp. 21-28.