La importancia de ser moderno. Problemas de método e ideología en el debate sobre la cognición y la conducta de los neandertales
- Balari, Sergi
- Benítez Burraco, Antonio
- Camps Serra, Marta
- Longa Martínez, Víctor Manuel
- Lorenzo González, Guillermo José
ISSN: 1133-5165
Ano de publicación: 2010
Volume: 18
Número: 34
Páxinas: 143-170
Tipo: Artigo
Outras publicacións en: Ludus vitalis: revista de filosofía de las ciencias de la vida = journal of philosophy of life sciences = revue de philosophie des sciences de la vie
Resumo
Some scholars defend that Neanderthals displayed a fully modern behavior, and they contend that those who do not share such a view are biased by an anthropocentric stance. We argue that the defense of a cognitive and behavioral asymmetry between Neanderthals and anatomically modern humans does not purport embracing an ideologically grounded position. In addition, we aim to explain that a methodological agenda, which we refer to as `radical continuism,' underlies such a non-scientific derivation of the debate on recent human evolution.
Referencias bibliográficas
- Anderson, S.R. (2004), Doctor Dolittle's Delusión. Animals and the Uniqueness of Human Language. New Haven & London: Yale University Press.
- Ayala, F. y C.J. Cela Conde (2006), La piedra que se volvió palabra. Las claves evolutivas de la humanidad. Madrid: Temas de Hoy.
- Balari, S. y G. Lorenzo (2009), “¿Para qué sirve un ballestrinque? Reflexiones sobre el funcionamiento de artefactos y organismos en un mundo sin funciones” Ms. Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona/Universidad de Oviedo, en proceso de revisión para Teorema.
- Balari, S., A. Benítez Burraco, M. Camps, V.M. Longa, G. Lorenzo y J. Uriagereka (2008), “¿Homo loquens neanderthalensis? En torno a las capacidades simbólicas y lingüísticas del neandertal”, Munibe Antropologia-Arkeologia 59: 3-24.
- Bar-Yosef, O. (2002), “The Upper Paleolithic revolution,” Annual Review of Anthropology 31: 363-393.
- Benítez Burraco, A. (2009), Genes y lenguaje. Aspectos ontogenéticos, filogenéticos y cognitivos. Barcelona: Editorial Reverté.
- Benítez Burraco, A., V.M. Longa, G. Lorenzo y J. Uriagereka (2008), “Also Sprach Neanderthalis... or did She?”, Biolinguistics 2/2-3: 225-232.
- Bickerton, D. (1990), Language and Species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Hay trad. de M.A. Valladares, Lenguaje y especie. Madrid: Alianza, 1994.
- Bickerton, D. (1995), Language and Human Behavior. Seattle: University of Washington Press.
- Binford, L.R. (1989), Debating Archaeology. San Diego: Academic Press.
- Boule, M. (1911-1913), “L'homme fossile de la Chapelle-aux-Saints”, Annales de Paléontologie 6 (1911), 1-64, 7 (1912), 65-208, y 8 (1913), 209-279.
- Budiansky, S. (1998), If a Lion Could Talk. Animal Intelligence and the Evolution of Consciousness. New York: Free Press. Citamos por la trad. de B. Martín, Si los animales hablaran no les entenderíamos. La evolución de la conciencia y la inteligencia. Madrid: Ateles, 2001.
- Bybee, J. (1998), “A functionalist approach to grammar and its evolution,” Evolution of Communication 2/2: 249-278.
- Caird, R. [dir. científico, R. Foley] (1994), Ape man. The Story of Human Evolution. London: Boxtree Ltd. Citamos por la trad. de TEC, S.L., Hombre mono. La historia de la evolución humana. Barcelona: Tusquets & Fundació “La Caixa”, 1995.
- Camps, M. (2009), “Where there's a will there's a way? 30 years of debate on the Mid-Upper Paleolithic transition in western Europe,” in M. Camps y C. Szmidt (eds.), The Mediterranean from 50,000 to 25,000 BP. Turning Points and New Directions. Oxford: Oxbow, pp. 2-10.
- Carroll, S.B. (2005),Endless Forms Most Beautiful. The New Science of Evo Devo and the Making of Animal Kingdom. New York: W.W. Norton.
- Chomsky, N. (1968), Language and Mind. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Hay trad. de J. Ferraté y S. Oliva, El lenguaje y el entendimiento. Barcelona: Seix-Barral, 1971.
- Dart, R. (1957), The Osteodontokeratic Culture of Australopithecus Prometheus. Pretoria: Transvaal Museum.
- Davidson, I. (2003), “The archaeological evidence of language origins: States of art,” in M. Christiansen y S. Kirby (eds.), Language Evolution. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 140-157.
- Deacon, T. (1997), The Symbolic Species. The Co-evolution of Language and the Human Brain. London: Penguin.
- d'Errico, F. (2003), “The invisible frontier: A multispecies model for the origin of behavioral modernity,” Evolutionary Anthropology 12: 188-202.
- d'Errico, F., J. Zilhão, M. Julien, D. Baffier y J. Pelegrin (1998a), “Neandertal acculturation in Western Europe? A critical review of the evidence and its interpretation,” Current Anthropology 39/2, Supplement: S1-S22. Special issue: The Neanderthal problem and the evolution of human behavior (comentarios y respuesta de los autores: S22-S44).
- d'Errico, F., C. Henshilwood, M. Vanhaeren y K. van Niekerk (2005), “Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave: evidence for symbolic behaviour in the Middle Stone Age,” Journal of Human Evolution 48: 3-24.
- d'Errico, F. y M. Vanhaeren (2007), “Evolution or revolution? New evidence for the origin of symbolic behavior in and out of Africa,” in P. Mellars, K. Boyle, O. Bar-Yosef y C. Stringer (eds.), Rethinking the Human Revolution: New Behavioural And Biological Perspectives on the Origin and Dispersal of Modern Humans. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, pp. 275-286.
- Dessalles, J.-L. (2000), Aux origins du langage. Une histoire naturelle de la parole. Paris: Hermes.
- Downes, S. (2003), “Baldwin effects and the expansion of explanatory repertoire in evolutionary biology,” in B. Weber y D. Depew (eds.), Evolution and Learning. The Baldwin Effect Reconsidered. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, pp. 33-51.
- Foley, R. (1987), Another Unique Species. New York: Longman.
- Foley, R. (2006), “The emergence of culture in the context of hominin evolutionary patterns,” in S.C. Levinson y P. Jaisson (eds.), Evolution and Culture. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 53-77.
- Gamble, C. (1999), The Palaeolithic Societies of Europe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Graves, P. (1991), “New models and metaphors for the Neanderthal debate,” Current Anthropology 32/5: 513-525 (comentarios y respuesta del autor: 525-541).
- Guthrie, R.D. (2005), The Nature of Paleolithic Art. Chicago & London: The University of Chicago Press.
- Györi, G. (1995), “Animal communication and human language: Searching for their evolutionary relationship,” in S. Puppel (ed.), The Biology of Language. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 99-126.
- Hall, B.K. (1999), Evolutionary Developmental Biology, 2nd ed. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
- Hauser, M.D. (1996): The Evolution of Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Hauser, M.D., N. Chomsky y W.T. Fitch (2002), “The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve?”, Science 298: 1569-1579.
- Henshilwood, C.S., F. d'Errico, R. Yates, Z. Jacobs, C. Tribolo, G. Duller, N. Mercier, J.C. Sealy, H. Valladas, I. Watts y A.G. Wintle (2002), “Emergence of modern human behaviour: Middle Stone Age engravings from South Africa,” Science 295: 1278-1280.
- Henshilwood, C.S. y C.W. Marean (2003), “The origin of modern human behavior. Critique of the models and their test implications,” Current Anthropology 44: 627-651.
- Hill, J. (1978), “Apes and language,” Annual Review of Anthropology 7: 89-112.
- Hublin, J.-J. (1998), “Comment on d'Errico, et al. 1998, `Neandertal acculturation in Western Europe?'”, Current Anthropology 39/2: S24-S25. Special issue: The Neanderthal problem and the evolution of human behavior.
- Jacob, F. (1997), La souris, la miuche et l'homme. Paris: Odile Jacob. Hay trad. de A. Martínez, El ratón, la mosca y el hombre. Barcelona: Crítica, 1998.
- Kauffman, S. (1995), At Home in the Universe. The Search for the Laws of Self-Organization and Complexity. New York: Oxford University Press.
- Klein, R.G. (2009), The Human Career. Human Biological and Cultural Origins. 3rd ed. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
- Leakey, R. (1981), The Making of Mankind. New York: E.P. Dutton. Citamos por la trad. de M. Domingo, La formación de la humanidad. Barcelona: Óptima/Del Serbal, 1981.
- Lewis-Williams, D. (2002), The Mind in the Cave. Consciousness and the Origins of Art. London: Thames & Hudson. Citamos por la trad. de E. Herrando, La mente en la caverna. La conciencia y los orígenes del arte. Madrid: Akal, 2005.
- Lewis-Williams, D. (2009), “Of people and pictures: the nexus of Upper Palaeolithic religion, social discrimination, and art,” in C. Renfrew e I. Morley (eds.), Becoming Human. Innovation in Prehistoric Material and Spiritual Culture, Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, pp. 135-158.
- Longa, V.M. (2007), “Sobre la relación entre el lenguaje y la comunicación animal”, Moenia 13: 5-37.
- Lorenzo, G. (2006), El vacío sexual, la tautología natural y la promesa minimalista. Madrid: Antonio Machado Libros.
- McBrearty, S. y A.S. Brooks (2000), “The revolution that wasn't: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behaviour,” Journal of Human Evolution 39: 453-563.
- Mellars, P. (1996), “Symbolism, language, and the Neanderthal mind,” in P. Mellars y K. Gibson (eds.), Modelling the Early Human Mind. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, pp. 15-32.
- Mellars, P. (1998a), “Neanderthals, modern humans and the archaeological evidence for language,” in N. Jablonski y L.C. Aiello (eds.), The Origin and Diversification of Language. San Francisco: California Academy of Sciences, pp. 89-115.
- Mellars, P. (1998b), “Comment on d'Errico, et al. 1998, `Neandertal acculturation in Western Europe?'”, Current Anthropology 39/2: S25-S26. Special issue: The Neanderthal problem and the evolution of human behavior.
- Mellars, P. (2002), “Archaeology and the origins of modern humans: European and African perspectives,” in T.J. Crow (ed.), The Speciation of Modern Homo sapiens. Proceedings of the British Academy 106. Oxford & New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 31-47. Citamos por la trad. de N. Cases, A. Martínez y L. Ríos, “Arqueología y los orígenes de los humanos modernos: perspectiva europea y africana,” in T.J. Crow (ed.), La especiación del Homo sapiens moderno. Madrid: Triacastela, 2005, pp. 45-62.
- Mellars, P. (2005), “The impossible coincidence. A single-species model for the origins of modern human behavior in Europe,” Evolutionary Anthropology 14: 12-27.
- Mithen, S. (1996), The Prehistory of the Mind. A Search for the Origins of Art, Religion, and Science. London: Thames & Hudson. Hay trad. de Mª.J. Aubet, Arqueología de la mente. Orígenes del arte, de la religión y de la ciencia. Barcelona: Crítica, 1998.
- Mithen, S. (2006), The Singing Neanderthals. The Origins of Music, Language, Mind and Body. London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson. Citamos por la trad. de G. Djembé, Los neandertales cantaban rap. Los orígenes de la música y el lenguaje. Barcelona: Crítica, 2007.
- Mithen, S. (2007a), “How the evolution of the human mind might be reconstructed,” in S.W. Gangestad y J.A. Simpson (eds.), The Evolution of Mind. Fundamental Questions and Controversies. New York and London: The Guilford Press, pp. 60-66.
- Mithen, S. (2007b), “General intellectual ability,” in S.W. Gangestad y J.A. Simpson (eds.), The Evolution of Mind. Fundamental Questions and Controversies. New York and London: The Guilford Press, pp. 319-324.
- Noble, W. e I. Davidson (1991), “The evolutionary emergence of modern human behaviour: Language and its archaeology,” Man (New Series) 26/2: 223-253.
- Noble, W. e I. Davidson (1996), Human Evolution, Language and Mind: a Psychological and Archaeological Inquiry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Proctor, R.N. (2007), “Material metaphors. Review of Origins and Revolutions: Human Identity in Earliest Prehistory by Clive Gamble,” Nature 448: 752-753.
- Reid, R.G.B. (2007), Biological Emergences. Evolution by Natural Experiment. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
- Rondal, J.-A. (2000), Le langage: de l'animal aux origines du langage humain. Sprimont: Mardaga.
- Savage-Rumbaugh, S. y R. Lewin (1994), Kenzi. The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
- Savage-Rumbaugh, S., G.S. Shanker y T.J. Taylor (1998), Apes, Language, and the Human Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Searle, J.R. (1995), The Construction of Social Reality, New York: The Free Press. Hay trad. de A. Domènech, La construcción de la realidad social, Barcelona: Paidós, 1997.
- Solecki, R. (1971), Shanidar — The First Flower People. New York: Knopf.
- Straus, L.G. (2003), “'The Aurignacian?' Some thoughts,” in J. Zilhão y F. d'Errico (eds.), The Chronology of the Aurignacian and of the Transicional Technocomplexes. Dating, Stratigraphies, Cultural Implications (Proceedings of Symposium 6.1. of the XIVth Congress of the UISSP, University of Liège, Belgium, September, 2001), Trabalhos de Arqueologia 33: 11-17.
- Straus, W.L. y A.J.E. Cave (1957), “Pathology and the posture of Neanderthal man,” Quarterly Review of Biology32/4: 348-363.
- Striedter, G.F. (2005), Principles of Brain Evolution. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
- Stringer, C. (2001), “Modern human origins—Distinguishing the models,” African Archaeological Review 18/2: 67-75.
- Stringer, C. (2002), “New perspectives on the Neanderthals,” Evolutionary Anthropology, Supplement 1: 58-59.
- Stringer, C. y C. Gamble (1993), In Search of the Neandertals. Solving the Puzzle of Human Origins. London: Thames & Hudson. Citamos por la trad. de O. Canals, En busca de los Neandertales: la solución al rompecabezas de los orígenes humanos. Barcelona: Crítica, 1996.
- Tattersall, I. (1998), Becoming Human: Evolution and Human Uniqueness. New York: Harcourt Brace & Co. Citamos por la trad. de J. Solé, Hacia el ser humano. La singularidad del hombre y la evolución. Barcelona: Península, 1998.
- Tattersall, I. (2008), The World From Beginnings to 4000 BCE. New York: Oxford University Press.
- Thorne, A. y M. Wolpoff (1992), “The multiregional evolution of modern humans,” Scientific American 266: 76-83.
- Tobias, P. (1983), “Recent advances in the evolution of the hominids with special reference to brain and speech,” Pontificial Academy of Sciences. Scripta Varia 50: 85-140.
- Tomasello, M. (2003), Constructing a Language. A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
- Trinkaus, E. (2007), “Human evolution: Neandertal gene speaks out,” Current Biology 17: R917-R918.
- Trinkaus, E. y P. Shipman (1993), The Neandertals: Changing the Image of Mankind. London: Jonathan Cape.
- Vega Toscano, L.G. (2007), “Esos enigmáticos neandertales. Historia de un debate sobre nuestros orígenes,” en L. Raposo, L.G. Vega Toscano, F. d'Errico, A. Turq, M. Mussi, E. Carbonell y G.-C. Weniger, El universo Neanderthal I. Madrid y Soria: Ibersaf & Fundación Duques de Soria, pp. 59-79.
- Weaver, T.D. y J.J. Hublin (2009), “Neanderthal birth canal shape and the evolution of human childbirth,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106/20: 8151.
- Wynn, T. y F.L. Coolidge (2004), “The expert Neandertal mind,” Journal of Human Evolution 46: 467-487.
- Zilhão, J. (2001), Anatomically Archaic, Behaviorally Modern: The Last Neanderthals and their Destiny. Stichting Nederlands Museum voor Anthropologie en Praehistoriae.
- Zilhão, J. (2003), “Comment on Henshilwood and Marean 2003, `The origin of modern human behaviour. Critique of the models and their test implications,'” Current Anthropology 44: 642-643.
- Zilhão, J. y F. d'Errico (1999), “The chronology and taphonomy of the earliest Aurignacian and its implications for the understanding of Neandertal extinction,” Journal of World Prehi